Class & Health

Michael Pollak mpollak at panix.com
Tue Jun 1 03:47:14 PDT 1999


[I think it was only this Fall Doug was remarking that there was a paucity of research on this subject. But now there seems to be an explosion, at least in the professional journals. And their conclusions are remarkably strong compared to what went before according to this NYT survey.]

Michael

For Good Health, It Helps to Be Rich and Important

By ERICA GOODE

D octors usually evaluate patients' vulnerability to serious

disease by inquiring about risk factors like cigarette smoking,

obesity, hypertension and high cholesterol.

But they might be better off asking how much money those patients

make, how many years they spent in school and where they stand

relative to others in their offices and communities.

_________________________________________________________________

Trying to explain why illness diminishes with a rise in social class.

_________________________________________________________________

Scientists have known for decades that poverty translates into

higher rates of illness and mortality. But an explosion of research

is demonstrating that social class -- as measured not just by

income but also by education and other markers of relative status

-- is one of the most powerful predictors of health, more powerful

than genetics, exposure to carcinogens, even smoking.

What matters is not simply whether a person is rich or poor,

college educated or not. Rather, risk for a wide variety of

illnesses, including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, arthritis,

infant mortality, many infectious diseases and some types of

cancer, varies with relative wealth or poverty: the higher the rung

on the socioeconomic ladder, the lower the risk. And this

relationship holds even at the upper reaches of society, where it

might seem that an abundance of resources would even things out.

Social class is an uncomfortable subject for many Americans. "I

think there has been a resistance to thinking about social

stratification in our society," said Dr. Nancy Adler, professor of

medical psychology at the University of California at San Francisco

and director of the John T. and Catherine D. MacArthur Foundation

Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health. Instead,

researchers traditionally have focused on health differences

between rich and poor, or blacks and whites (unaware, in many

cases, that race often served as a proxy for socioeconomic status,

since blacks are disproportionately represented in lower income

brackets).

But the notion that a mid-level executive with a three-bedroom,

split-level in Scarsdale might somehow be more vulnerable to

illness than his boss in the five-bedroom colonial a few blocks

away seems to have finally captured scientists' attention.

In the past five years, 193 papers addressing aspects of

socioeconomic status and health have appeared in scientific

journals -- twice the number in the previous five-year period. The

National Institutes of Health last year declared research on

disparities in health related to social class or minority status

one of its highest priorities, said Dr. Norman Anderson, associate

director of the N.I.H. And a recent conference in Bethesda, Md., on

the topic, sponsored by the New York Academy of Sciences and the

MacArthur Foundation network, drew more than 250 participants from

a wide variety of disciplines.

Executive Privilege Includes Longevity

A study following 18,133 male civil servants over 25 years shows that

the grade of employment was a strong predictor of mortality,

especially before retirement.

[060199hth-socioeconomic-status.1.gif]

The New York Times

Source: M. G. Marmot and Martin J. Shipley.

_________________________________________________________________

What first compelled researchers interest was a now-classic study,

begun in the late 1960's, of men in the British civil service. The

Whitehall study, directed by Dr. Michael Marmot, director of the

International Center for Health and Society at University College

London, tracked mortality rates over 10 years for 17,530 male civil

service employees.

When the data were analyzed, the researchers were astonished to

discover that mortality rates varied continuously and precisely

with the men's civil service grade: the higher the classification,

the lower the rates of death, regardless of cause.

This finding was as perplexing as it was intriguing. The men all

had jobs, and equal access to health care under Britain's national

health system. But mortality rates for men in the lowest civil

service classification, the researchers found, were three times

higher than those for men in the highest grade. And a 25-year

follow-up of the Whitehall subjects, some results of which were

published in 1996, found the social class gradient persisted well

past retirement, even among men into their late 80's.

Subsequent studies demonstrated a similar relationship between

socioeconomic status and mortality in the United States and Canada.

What could account for such startling findings? One plausible

explanation was that lower-ranked men might engage in more risky

behaviors, like smoking.

But the same health disparity from pay grade to pay grade that was

evident in smokers held for nonsmokers, too. And all coronary risk

factors combined, the researchers found, accounted for only a third

of the differences between grades.

Stress, or other aspects of psychological life that can have an

impact on a person's vulnerability to disease, the Whitehall group

reasoned, might also play a role in the results. Prolonged exposure

to stress, researchers have found, can lead to abnormalities in

immune function and glucose metabolism, and destroy brain cells

involved in memory. And studies show that the lower one's social

status, the more stressed people feel, and the more stressful

events they encounter in their lives.

Dr. Sheldon Cohen, professor of psychology at Carnegie Mellon

University in Pittsburgh, has demonstrated a link between social

status and vulnerability to infectious disease in male macaque

monkeys. In a study carried out in conjunction with primate

researchers at Wake Forest University School of Medicine, Dr. Cohen

found that males at the lower end of the dominance hierarchy were

more susceptible to a cold virus than dominant males.

Dr. Cohen and his colleagues at Carnegie Mellon then replicated

those findings in humans. Subjects in the study were asked to rate

their relative standing in their community on a social status

ladder, then were exposed to mild respiratory virus. People who

ranked themselves low on the ladder were more likely to become

infected with the virus than those who ranked themselves higher up

on the ladder.

In another study, the researchers found that people who had been

unemployed for one month or more under highly stressful conditions

were 3.8 times more susceptible to a virus than people who were not

experiencing a significant stressful situation.

At least in primates, the interaction of stress with social class

and illness, however, depends both on the nature of the stress and

the context in which it occurs, as demonstrated in a series of

studies by Dr. Jay R. Kaplan, a professor of pathology and

anthropology at Wake Forest.

In a crowded situation where resources are scarce, for example,

male monkeys at the lowest end of the dominance hierarchy, who must

scramble hardest to survive, are likely to feel the most stress.

And in studies of primates in the wild, researchers find that

subordinate animals show higher levels of stress hormones.

But when Dr. Kaplan and his colleagues fed male monkeys a "luxury"

diet, high in fat and cholesterol, and moved them each month for

two years into a new group of strange males, it was the dominant

animals, forced to reassert their position continually, who

suffered the most stress. Under such conditions, Dr. Kaplan finds,

dominant males show a hypervigilant response, and have higher rates

of coronary artery disease than subordinates.

For humans and primates, a sense of control over life events is

intimately related to stress. And control seems to have been one

factor at work in the Whitehall study.

In 1985, Dr. Marmot and his colleagues launched Whitehall II, a

second large-scale study, which included civil servants of both

sexes and which collected more detailed information on the

participants.

As part of the study, employees were asked to rate the amount of

control they felt over their jobs. Managers also rated the amount

of control employees had. Job control, the researchers found,

varied inversely with employment grade: the higher the grade, the

more control. And the less control employees had, as defined either

by their own or managers' ratings, the higher the employees' risk

of developing coronary disease. Job control, in fact, accounted for

about half the gradient in deaths from pay grade to pay grade.

To some extent, people's ability to withstand stress, and the ways

in which they interpret and respond to life events, are shaped by

early life, the product of what one social scientist, Dr. Clyde

Hertzman of the University of British Columbia, calls "the long arm

of childhood." Genetics play a role, as does nutrition. (In the

Whitehall study, height -- which varies with social class -- was

used as a rough indicator of childhood influences on development,

and accounted for a small portion of the association between

mortality and employment grade.) And scientists have found that

early life experiences -- abuse and neglect, for example -- can

alter brain development and influence responses to stressful

events.

Any discussion of socioeconomic status in the United States of

necessity involves a discussion of race, since the two are entwined

in complex, sometimes inextricable, ways. Proportionally, far more

African-Americans live in poverty than whites: 28.4 percent of

blacks fell below the poverty line in 1996, compared with 11.2

percent of whites, according to Government data.

Death rates for African-Americans from all causes are 1.6 times

higher than for white Americans, Dr. David R. Williams of the

University of Michigan's Institute for Social Research, said at the

Bethesda conference.

Life expectancy for blacks and whites also varies. At age 45, a

white man can expect to live five years longer than an

African-American man, and white women can expect to live 3.7 years

longer than their black counterparts.

If socioeconomic status is taken into account, health differences

between blacks and whites decrease substantially: Black men in the

highest income brackets, for example, have a life expectancy 7.4

years longer than black men in the lowest brackets, Dr. Williams

said. White men at top income levels live 6.6 years longer than

their lowest-income counterparts.

But race and to some extent sex still have an impact on health that

is independent of social class. The gap in infant mortality rates

between blacks and whites, for example, actually increases with

higher social status. And being black or female discounts some of

the advantages afforded by education: white men accrue health

advantages with every additional year of schooling they receive.

But black men and women, though they also show gains, show them

only through high school, according to an analysis of Federal data

by Dr. Adler and Dr. Burton Singer of Princeton University's Office

of Population Research. White women, the researchers found,

continue to gain in health status through college, but unlike white

men, do not receive the gains in health bestowed by post-graduate

education.

Social exclusion, residential segregation and other expressions of

institutional racism magnify the impact of socioeconomic status.

Several studies, for example, have shown higher adult and infant

mortality rates for people living in segregated areas.

For both blacks and whites, living in a neighborhood where social

bonds have eroded may have negative effects on health. Dr. Robert

Putnam of Harvard University coined the term "social capital" to

describe the elements that contribute to social cohesion.

Dr. Ichiro Kawachi, director of the Harvard Center for Society and

Health, has explored one aspect of social capital -- interpersonal

trust -- and its relationship to national and community rates of

illness and death.

Dr. Kawachi and his colleagues correlated mortality rates in states

with the percentage of state residents who agreed with the

statement, "Most people would try to take advantage of you if they

got the chance." Death rates, Dr. Kawachi reported at the Bethesda

conference, were strongly linked to level of social trust, with the

most mistrustful ratings clustering in southern and northeastern

states. Another study, of Chicago neighborhoods, yielded similar

findings: neighborhoods in which more residents agreed with the

statement "Neighbors can be trusted" had lower mortality rates.

Why are neighborhoods or states with higher levels of trust

healthier? Dr. Kawachi suggests that in neighborhoods where social

trust is high, negative health behaviors -- smoking and alcohol

consumption, for example -- might be discouraged through community

pressure.

Residents in high trust neighborhoods may also share more

resources, be more willing to help one another out and offer one

another more emotional support. "It is speculation," Dr. Kawachi

said, "but probably these little things add up to quite important

health differences."

Even so, the relationship between social class and health is

unlikely to be entirely explained by social capital, or by any

other single dimension of life experience. Said Dr. Adler, "There

isn't going to be a single explanation or an easy solution, but

we've started mapping out some of the places where we can

intervene."

_________________________________________________________________

Copyright 1999 The New York Times Company



More information about the lbo-talk mailing list