More on False Memories

Kevin Robert Dean qualiall_2 at yahoo.com
Mon Jul 2 17:40:29 PDT 2001


American Psychological Association (APA) 2-Jul-01

http://www.newswise.com/articles/2001/7/REMEMBER.PSY.html

People Think They Remember Library: LIF-SOC Keywords: MEMORY ILLUSIONS INFERENCE EYEWITNESS COURT Description: Memory "illusions" may result from the basic human need to make sense out of events. A series of experiments has provided the first scientific evidence that when people see effects without also seeing its cause, they automatically "fill in the blank" with that probable cause. (J. of Experimental Psychology, Jul-2001)

Contact: Public Affairs Office (202) 336-5700 public.affairs at apa.org

EMBARGO: NOT FOR RELEASE UNTIL 6:00 PM (EDT), JULY 1, 2001

PEOPLE MAKE SENSE OUT OF STAND ALONE EFFECTS BY THINKING THEY "REMEMBER" SEEING THEIR PROBABLE CAUSES

The need for a coherent world can cause mistaken memories based on "causal inference" errors, with implications for eyewitness testimony

WASHINGTON -- Memory "illusions" may result from the basic human need to make sense out of events. A series of experiments has provided the first scientific evidence that when people see effects (a student toppling onto the floor) without also seeing its cause (a student leaning back in a chair), they automatically "fill in the blank" with that probable cause -- even if they haven't actually seen it with their own two eyes. The result: a memory that seems real, but isn't. The inference may be correct, but it's not based on actual perception, suggesting that memory helps us to make sense of the world, perhaps at the expense of a complete reliability. The findings are detailed in the July issue of the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, published by the American Psychological Association.

Psychologists Sharon L. Hannigan, Ph.D., of Bard College and Mark Tippens Reinitz, Ph.D., of the University of Puget Sound conducted three experiments (with 144 subjects in the first two and 48 in the third) that demonstrated "causal inference errors," which are memory errors that result when people make inferences about the underlying causes of events. Hannigan and Reinitz showed subjects pictures depicting some kind of "effect," such as oranges sprawled on a supermarket floor. They later showed pictures of the most probable cause of that effect, such as someone reaching for an orange from the bottom of a stack, and asked subjects whether they had seen that picture before. A statistically significant number said they did.

So, subjects filled in the gaps of missing scenes by saying they saw pictures they never actually saw -- pictures that might have been expected to have been there in the first place. They inferred correctly, but their underlying memories were illusions. Thus, normal memory can be "tricked" in the way that optical illusions "trick" (or manipulate) normal visual perception. This finding confirms causal-inference error as a new category of inference-based memory error. "It is surprising that just a few minutes after seeing the effect scene, people would reliably claim to have seen the cause scene," Reinitz comments. "After all, we tend to believe that we can accurately remember what we saw just a few minutes ago."

Because memory for pictures tends to be very accurate and robust, the experiments used pictorial stimuli, not the more typically studied text stimuli, as a more rigorous test of the error. What's more, "we put a lot of confidence in things that we have seen with our own eyes," says Reinitz, "so applications to real-world situations are probably more varied and interesting than would be the case if we used text."

Hannigan and Reinitz also found that errors increased with longer retention intervals. This finding has special importance for the courtroom, where cases go to trial many months after events occur -- making eyewitnesses even more prone to inference-based errors than they already are. Causal inferences could also play an important role in relationships, in that people can misremember the causes of others' behavior due to inference-based memory errors.

The researchers emphasize that their findings illuminate normal memory processes that are generally useful and adaptive. "More often than not," Reinitz reiterates, "causal inferences are likely to be correct."

Hannigan and Reinitz also found that these causal-inference errors were common in a backward, not forward direction -- that is, exposure to "effect" slides caused illusory memories of seeing "cause" slides, but exposure to "cause" slides did not cause illusory memories of seeing "effect" slides. In their article, the researchers discuss possible reasons why people didn't "fill in the blanks" when effect was missing, and call for additional research into this potentially significant phenomenon. Reinitz speculates that it may be due to a stronger primal need to ask, "Why?" than to ask "What would happen if.?"

Finally, the experimental results suggest that the underlying memory mechanisms that give rise to causal-inference errors are fundamentally different from those that give rise to schema-based memory errors, further evidence that "memory" is actually a collection of different, independent processes.

Article: "A Demonstration and Comparison of Two Types of Inference-Based Memory Errors," by Sharon L. Hannigan, Ph.D., Bard College and Mark Tippens Reinitz, Ph.D., University of Puget Sound; Journal of Experimental Psychology -- Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Vol 27. No.4.

Mark Tippens Reinitz can be reached by phone at (253) 879-3742 or by electronic mail at mreinitz at ups.edu.

Full text of the article is available from the APA Public Affairs Office and after July 6 at http://www.apa.org/journals/xlm/xlm274931.html

The American Psychological Association (APA), in Washington, DC, is the largest scientific and professional organization representing psychology in the United States and is the world's largest association of psychologists. APA's membership includes more than 155,000 researchers, educators, clinicians, consultants and students. Through its divisions in 55 divisions of psychology and affiliations with 60 state, territorial and Canadian provincial associations, APA works to advance psychology as a science, as a profession and as a means of promoting human welfare.

http://www.newswise.com/articles/2001/7/REMEMBER.PSY.html # # #

ooooooooo Kevin Dean Buffalo, NY ICQ # 8616001 http://www.yaysoft.com ooooo

_________________________________________________________ Do You Yahoo!? Get your free @yahoo.com address at http://mail.yahoo.com



More information about the lbo-talk mailing list